心理科学 2018, (2) 278-284 DOI:     ISSN: 0412-1961 CN: 21-1139/TG

本期目录 | 下期目录 | 过刊浏览 | 高级检索                                                            [打印本页]   [关闭]
基础、实验与工效
扩展功能
本文信息
Supporting info
PDF(651KB)
[HTML全文]
参考文献[PDF]
参考文献
服务与反馈
把本文推荐给朋友
加入我的书架
加入引用管理器
引用本文
Email Alert
文章反馈
浏览反馈信息
本文关键词相关文章
时间知觉 疼痛表情 唤醒 注意
本文作者相关文章
黄顺航
刘培朵
李庆庆
陈有国
黄希庭
PubMed
Article by Huang,S.H
Article by Liu,P.D
Article by Li,Q.Q
Article by Chen,W.G
Article by Huang,X.T
疼痛表情对秒下及秒上时距知觉的影响
黄顺航1,刘培朵2,2,李庆庆2,2,陈有国1,黄希庭1
1. 西南大学
2.
摘要: 采用二分法和泛化法范式,在秒下及秒上呈现时距探究疼痛表情对时距知觉的影响。结果发现,使用二分法,疼痛表情在秒上秒下都显著延长了个体的主观时距,但使用泛化法,疼痛表情仅在秒上产生影响。根据时间分段综合模型和范式特点,该实验结果反映疼痛表情对时距知觉的影响在秒上秒下的作用机制可能不同,在秒下主要是通过唤醒影响,在秒上则可能由唤醒和注意共同调节。
关键词时间知觉 疼痛表情 唤醒 注意  
The Influence of Facial Expressions of Pain on Subsecond and Suprasecond
Abstract: The aim of the current study was using a temporal generalization task and a temporal bisection task to explore whether facial pain could affect subsecond (200~800 ms) and suprasecond (1400- 2600 ms) time perception. Twenty six participants (fifteen males) participated in bisection task, which consisted of two blocks with different stimulus durations (200~800 ms vs. 1400~2600 ms), the order of them was counterbalanced across participants. In the block with 200~800 ms durations, the block consisted of a learning phase which used a “short” (200 ms) and a “long” (800 ms) anchor duration, a traing phase and a test phase involving seven probe durations (400, 600, 800, 1000, 1200, 1400 and 1600 ms). The participants were initially presented with the “short” and “long” anchor durations in the form of a light brown rectangle randomly. Then they were trained to press D/K keys to indicate whether the presentation duration of the rectangle was more closer to “short” or “long” anchor duration. A feedback was given after each response. In the test phase, participants were told that the rectangle would be replaced by faces and no feedback presented. Each participant completed 140 trials presented randomly. In another block, the material and the procedure were similar to 200~800 ms condition, except using 1400 ms and 2600ms as “short” and “long” standard duration and 1400,1600,1800,2000,2200,2400,2600 ms as comparison durations. To verify whether the effect of facial pain on time perception was stable. Other twenty six participants (nine males) took part in generalization task, which used a basically same procedure and materials as the time bisection task, without the standard durations was replaced to 500 ms in 200~800 ms condition and 2000 ms in 1400~2600 ms condition, as the demand was changed to judge whether the present duration was "same" or "different" to the standard duration. In temporal bisection task, the results pertaining to PSE revealed a significant main effect of duration , a significant main effect of facial expression, and a significant duration × face interaction, Subsequent Simple-effects Analysis showed that, in subsecond range, the PSE on facial pain was significantly lower than facial neutral, and in suprasecond range, the PSE on facial pain was also significantly lower than facial neutral. The analysis of WR just found a significant main effect of duration. The results tell a subjective lengthening effect produced by the perception of pain faces in both subsecond range and suprasecond range. In tempotal generalization task, the results pertaining to PSE revealed a significant main effect of duration and a significant main effect of facial expression, while the duration×face interaction was nonsignificant. A Paired Samples t-test showed that, in suprasecond range, the PSE on facial pain was significantly lower than facial neutral, while no significant difference was found between pain and neutral faces in subsecond range. The analysis of WR just found a significant main effect of duration. The results suggested a subjective lengthening effect produced by the perception of pain faces in suprasecond duration. It could be speculated that, in temporal bisection task, facial pain could accelerate the rate of the pacemaker because of its high arousal, and make the switch close quickly or open lately, or keep it at a relatively stable closed state, resulting a relatively long subjective duration. While in temporal generalization task, the effect of pain expression on temporal perception was just found in suprasecond range, it may because the arousal effect on timing was diminished as this task demands subjects remember the standard duration. Considering that different contribution of arousal and attention will change based on the time elapsed since the onset of the emotional event. These results suggest a major role of arousal mechanisms involving in subsecond time perception and both arousal and attention mechanisms in suprasecond time perception in the effect of facial pain expressions on time perception.
Keywords:
DOI:
基金项目:

中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金(SWU1509450)资助.;国家自然科学基金项目(31600879)资助;2016年“重庆市博士后科研项目特别资助

通讯作者: 黄希庭
作者简介: 2017-06-30
作者Email: xthuang@swu.edu.cn

参考文献:
黄希庭, 徐光国. (1999). 对变化/分割模型的检验 (II). 心理学报, 31(2), 135-141. 黄希庭, 李伯约, 张志杰. (2003). 时间认知分段综合模型的探讨. 西南师范大学学报: 人文社会科学版, 29(2), 5-9. 尹华站, 李祚山, 李丹, 黄希庭. (2010). 时距加工“长度效应”研究述评. 心理科学进展, 18(6), 887-891. Allman, M. J., Teki, S., Griffiths, T. D., & Meck, W. H. (2014). Properties of the internal clock: first- and second-order principles of subjective time. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 743–71. Baum, C., Schneider, R., Keogh, E., & Lautenbacher, S. (2013). Different stages in attentional processing of facial expressions of pain: A dot-probe task modification. Journal of Pain, 14(3), 223–232. Benuzzi, F., Lui, F., Duzzi, D., Nichelli, P. F., & Porro, C. A. (2008). Does it look painful or disgusting? Ask your parietal and cingulate cortex. J Neurosci, 28(4), 923–931. Buhusi, C. V, & Meck, W. H. (2009). Relative time sharing: new findings and an extension of the resource allocation model of temporal processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1875–1885. Cui, F., Abdelgabar, A. R., Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2015). Responsibility modulates pain-matrix activation elicited by the expressions of others in pain. NeuroImage, 114, 371–378. Droit-Volet, S., & Gil, S. (2009). The time-emotion paradox. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1943–53. Droit-Volet, S., & Meck, W. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(12), 504–513. Fayolle, S., Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2015). Fear and time: Fear speeds up the internal clock. Behavioural Processes, 120(November), 135–140. Fayolle, S. L., & Droit-Volet, S. (2014). Time perception and dynamics of facial expressions of emotions. PLoS ONE, 9(5). Fortin, C., Fairhurst, S., Malapani, C., Morin, C., Towey, J., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Expectancy in humans in multisecond peak-interval timing with gaps. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71(4), 789–802. Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2011). “Time flies in the presence of angry faces”... depending on the temporal task used! Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 354–362. Grommet, E. K., Droit-Volet, S., Gil, S., Hemmes, N. S., Baker, A. H., & Brown, B. L. (2011). Time estimation of fear cues in human observers. Behavioural Processes, 86(1), 88–93. Hayashi, M. J., Kantele, M., Walsh, V., Carlson, S., & Kanai, R. (2014). Dissociable Neuroanatomical Correlates of Subsecond and Suprasecond Time Perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(8), 1685–1693. Hellstr?m, C., & Carlsson, S. G. (1997). Busy with pain: Disorganization in subjective time in experimental pain. European Journal of Pain, 1(2), 133–139. Johnston, J. T. ? V. B. ? P. (2011). Facial Emotion Modulates the Neural Mechanisms Responsible for Short Interval Time Perception. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 5, 77. Kappesser, J., & Williams, A. C. de C. (2002). Pain and negative emotions in the face: Judgements by health care professionals. Pain, 99(1–2), 197–206. Lake, J. I., LaBar, K. S., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Emotional modulation of interval timing and time perception. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 403–420. Lamm, C., Decety, J., & Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. NeuroImage, 54(3), 2492–2502. Lui, M. A., Penney, T. B., & Schirmer, A. (2011). Emotion effects on timing: Attention versus pacemaker accounts. PLoS ONE, 6(7). Ogden, R. S., Moore, D., Redfern, L., & McGlone, F. (2014). The effect of pain and the anticipation of pain on temporal perception: A role for attention and arousal. Cognition and Emotion, 29(5), 910–922. Rattat, A.-C., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). What is the best and easiest method of preventing counting. Behavior Research, 44, 67–80. Sheng, F., & Han, S. (2012). Manipulations of cognitive strategies and intergroup relationships reduce the racial bias in empathic neural responses. NeuroImage, 61(4), 786–797. Simon, D., Craig, K. D., Gosselin, F., Belin, P., & Rainville, P. (2008). Recognition and discrimination of prototypical dynamic expressions of pain and emotions. Pain, 135(1–2), 55–64. Tipples, J. (2008). Negative emotionality influences the effects of emotion on time perception. Emotion, 8(1), 127–131. Tipples, J., Meck, W. H., Cheng, R.-K., & Narayanan, N. S. (2016). Clock Speed as a Window into Dopaminergic Control of Emotion and Time Perception. Timing & Time Perception, 4(1), 99–122. Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19(6), 705–743. Williams, A. C. D. C. (2002). Facial expression of pain: an evolutionary account. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(4), 439-455-488.
本刊中的类似文章

Copyright by 心理科学