›› 2019, Vol. ›› Issue (4): 834-840.

• 发展与教育 • 上一篇    下一篇

空格减少汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的视觉拥挤效应:来自眼动的证据

王敬欣1,李莎1,郝立莎1,张雪1,张阔2   

  1. 1. 天津师范大学心理与行为研究院
    2. 南开大学
  • 收稿日期:2018-07-04 修回日期:2018-10-23 出版日期:2019-07-20 发布日期:2019-07-20
  • 通讯作者: 张阔

Inserting Spaces Benefits Chinese Developmental Dyslexic Children for Reducing their Visual Crowding: Evidence from Eye Movements

  • Received:2018-07-04 Revised:2018-10-23 Online:2019-07-20 Published:2019-07-20

摘要: 使用眼动仪记录汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童、正常年龄匹配和能力匹配儿童阅读插入空格文本时的眼动,考察在字、词以及非词间插入空格呈现文本对汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童阅读加工过程的影响。结果发现,阅读障碍儿童在字间空格条件下平均注视时间减少的程度大于正常儿童,注视次数在字间和词间空格条件下未见显著增加,能力匹配儿童的注视次数在这两种空格条件下却显著增加。结果说明,在字间和词间插入空格能够减少儿童的平均阅读时间,且字间空格文本呈现显著地提高了其阅读效率,说明空格对汉语阅读障碍儿童具有促进作用,这种促进主要是由于空格减少了其视觉拥挤效应所致。

关键词: 发展性阅读障碍, 空格, 拥挤效应, 眼动

Abstract: In alphabetic languages, some researchers suggested that, increasing the size of the space between the letters and words in text can facilitate dyslexic children’ reading performance by reducing the effects of visual crowding. However, the text in Chinese is usually a sequence of box-like pictorial symbols (characters), and these characters often combine with adjacent characters to form a word. Particularly, the boundaries between words are not demarcated using spaces or other visual cues. Some previous research suggested that adding spaces between Chinese words can facilitate the performance of typically developing readers and second-language learners. But the potential benefits of adding spaces to text for Chinese dyslexic children are still unknown. Accordingly, in the present study, we investigated the benefits for developing dyslexic children when they read the sentences which were inserted spaces between the characters or words. Eleven dyslexic readers (fifth grade), 15 age-matched controls (typically developing fifth graders) and 13 reading ability-matched controls (typical developing third graders) from a primary school, screened by five tests, including text comprehension, literacy-capacity test, orthography test, phonetic transcription test, and rapid naming test, took part in the current study. Texts were presented normally, or with spaces between all characters, or between character pairs that combined to form either words or non-words. Eye movements were recorded by a SR research EyeLinkⅡ eye tracker while the children were reading. The results showed that inserting spaces between words or characters reduced fixation duration in comparison to normal text for readers from all groups. However, whereas for the age- and ability-matched groups this was largely offset by an increase in the number of fixations and regressions in these conditions, this was not the case for the dyslexic group, leading to a reduction in reading times in the character- and word-spaces conditions in comparison to the normal text for this group. Hence, compared to normal text, increased spacing showed no overall facilitation for typically-developing readers, and incurred a cost for all readers in the non-word condition. But clear facilitation was observed for dyslexic readers when spaces were added between characters or words in sentences. Therefore, the results indicate, for the first time, that increased character spacing can benefit the reading performance of Chinese readers with developmental dyslexia. This benefit may arise from the reduced effects of visual crowding.

Key words: Developmental Dyslexia, Space, Visual Crowding, Eye movements