心理科学 ›› 2013, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (1): 20-26.

• 基础、实验与工效 • 上一篇    下一篇

英语句法歧义消解过程中基于激发点的权变模型

韩迎春1,莫雷2   

  1. 1. 广东第二师范学院教育系
    2. 广东省广州市石牌华南师范大学
  • 收稿日期:2011-04-08 修回日期:2012-02-09 出版日期:2013-01-20 发布日期:2013-02-26
  • 通讯作者: 莫雷
  • 基金资助:
    国家自然科学基金资助项目;教育部哲学社会科学研究重大研究项目资助;广东省自然科学基金团队项目

Motivator-based contingency model in the resolution of English Syntactic ambiguity

HAN YingChun1,   

  • Received:2011-04-08 Revised:2012-02-09 Online:2013-01-20 Published:2013-02-26

摘要: 采用自控速度的移动窗口技术,在3个实验中分别考察句法歧义消解的基本过程。阅读材料为 不含有词汇歧义的较为单纯的英语句法歧义句。结果为:实验1的研究结果表明在语料中不存 在歧义激发点的情况下,被试不会启动歧义的消解过程;实验2的研究结果表明当歧义激发点 与解歧区重合时,人们会选用再分析机制来处理解歧区语料;实验3的研究结果表明当歧义激 发点与歧义区重合时,人们会选用竞争机制来处理歧义区语料。本研究初步验证了基于激发点 的权变模型关于句法歧义消解过程的相关设想。

关键词: 句法歧义, 再分析模型, 竞争模型, 激发点, 权变模型

Abstract: Two competing and largely incompatible classes of model dominate current sentence processing research. One is called Linear view. The best known account of this class is the garden-path model, in which the processor makes initial decisions on the basis of strategies defined in terms of syntactic information alone and uses thematic information (such as determined by minimal attachment and late closure).According to this class, the processor computes syntactic analyses serially, in two stages. In the first stage, it draws on a restricted range of information to construct an initial analysis. During the second stage, it accesses other sources of information, which may sometimes cause it to abandon its initial analysis and compute another. The second class of model assumes that the processor can activate multiple analyses in parallel. It employs both syntactic and nonsyntactic information in a single stage to foreground one analysis, but other analyses remain activated, which is called Parallel view or competing model. The best known account of this class is the constraint-based model. In the framework of the contingency model, which mechanism the subjects will explore to deal with the syntactic ambiguity corpus depends on the contingent properties of the ambiguity motivator. The so-called "ambiguity motivator" refers to the information point that promotes and forces people to make a choice between the alternative analysis. According to the contingent properties of the ambiguity motivator, there are three conditions in the resolution of syntactic ambiguity. In the first case, the ambiguity motivator does not appear in any position of the corpus. In this case, the individual will generate an analysis based on their syntactic knowledge, context and their preference and so on. They won’t attempt to do any deep processing to resolute the ambiguity. The mechanism people adopt to parse the ambiguity corpus is as same as the mechanism people apply to process the unambiguity corpus. In the second case, the ambiguity motivator appears in the region of the disambiguation region of the corpus. In this case, people will select the reanalysis mechanism to parse the corpus of the disambiguation and the subsequent regions. In the third case, the ambiguity motivator appears in the ambiguity region of the corpus. In this case, people will use the competition mechanism to parse the ambiguity corpus. In current paper, according to the motivator-based contingency model,three experiments were designed. In the three experiments, the English syntactic ambiguity corpus was used to investigate the mechanism that people explored to process the ambiguity corpus in three conditions which composed of the absence of the ambiguity motivator, the ambiguity motivator in the disambiguation region and ambiguity motivator in the ambiguity region. The task was self-paced word-by-word reading with a moving window display. Each trial began with a red “+” on the central of the screen for 300ms. Participants pressed the spacebar to reveal each word of the sentence. As each new word appeared, the preceding word disappeared. The amount of time the participant spent reading each word was recorded as the time between key-presses. After the final word of each item, a comprehension question appeared which asked about information contained in the sentence just read. Participants pressed one of two keys of “J” or “F” to respond ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no.’’ After an incorrect answer, the word ‘‘INCORRECT’’ flashed briefly on the screen. No feedback was given for correct responses. Participants were asked to read sentences at a natural rate and to be sure that they understood what they read. They were told to answer the questions as quickly and accurately as they could and to take wrong answers as an indication to read more carefully. A T test of determiner (ambiguity, control) on these reading time data was conducted. The data of the three experiments showed that when the ambiguity motivator didn’t appear in any region of the corpus, people would apply the mechanism that they used to parse the unambiguity corpus to process syntactic ambiguity coupus. When the ambiguity motivator appeared in the disambiguation region, people would adopt the reanalysis mechanism to parse the corpus of the disambiguiation and the subsequent region. When the ambiguity motivator appeared in the ambiguity region, people would select the competition mechanism to parse the ambiguity corpus. The conclusion is the motivator-based contingency model is applicable to predict and explain the resolution of syntactic ambiguity. People will follow the contingent properties of the ambiguity motivator to select mechanism to process the ambiguity corpus.

Key words: syntactic ambiguity, reanalysis model, competing model, ambiguity motivator, contingency model