[1] 高文斌, 魏景汉, 彭小虎, 罗跃嘉. (2004). 位置提示下视觉注意范围的调控机制. 心理学报, 36(2), 139-144. [2] 何涛, 王治国. (2015). IOR的空间、视网膜和客体参照系. 心理科学, 38(4), 813-821. [3] 王爱君, 李毕琴, 张明. (2015). 三维空间深度位置上基于空间的IOR. 心理学报, 47(7), 859-868. [4] 王爱君, 刘晓乐, 唐晓雨, 张明. (2017). 三维空间中不同视野深度位置上的IOR. 心理学报, 49(6), 723-732. [5] Bauer D., Plinge A., Ehrenstein W. H., Rinkenauer G., & Grosjean M. (2012). Spatial orienting of attention in stereo depth. Psychological Research, 76(6), 730-735. [6] Bennett, P. J., & Pratt, J. (2001). The spatial distribution of inhibition of return. Psychological Science, 12(1), 76-80. [7] Bourke P. A., Partridge H., & Pollux, P. M. J. (2006). Additive effects of inhibiting attention to objects and locations in three-dimensional displays. Visual Cognition, 13(5), 643-654. [8] Casagrande M., Barbato M., Mereu S., Martella D., Marotta A., Theeuwes J., & Collinson S. L. (2012). Inhibition of return: A “depth-blind” mechanism? Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 75-80. [9] Downing, C. J., & Pinker, S. (1985). The spatial structure of visual attention. In M. I. Posner & O. S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 171-187). Erlbaum. [10] Eng V., Lim A., Janssen S. M. J., & Satel J. (2018). Time course of inhibition of return in a spatial cueing paradigm with distractors. Acta Psychologica, 183, 51-57. [11] Eriksen, C. W., & St James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception and Psychophysics, 40(4), 225-240. [12] Ghirardelli, T. G., & Folk, C. L. (1996). Spatial cuing in a stereoscopic display: Evidence for a "depth-blind" attentional spotlight. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3(1), 81-86. [13] Jordan, H., & Tipper, S. P. (1999). Spread of inhibition across an object' s surface. British Journal of Psychology, 90(4), 495-507. [14] Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 138-147. [15] McDonald J. J., Hickey C., Green J. J., & Whitman J. C. (2009). Inhibition of return in the covert deployment of attention: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(4), 725-733. [16] Pierce A. M., Crouse M. D., & Green J. J. (2017). Evidence for an attentional component of inhibition of return in visual search. Psychophysiology, 54(11), 1676-1685. [17] Plewan, T., & Rinkenauer, G. (2016). Fast and forceful: Modulation of response activation induced by shifts of perceived depth in virtual 3D space. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1939. [18] Plewan, T., & Rinkenauer, G. (2017). Simple reaction time and size-distance integration in virtual 3D space. Psychological Research, 81(3), 653-663. [19] Plewan, T., & Rinkenauer, G. (2018). The influence of relevant and irrelevant stereoscopic depth cues: Depth information does not always capture attention. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 80(8), 1996-2007. [20] Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma, & D.Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance. X(pp. 531-556). Erlbaum. [21] Redden R. S., Hilchey M. D., & Klein R. M. (2018). Oculomotor inhibition of return: Evidence against object-centered representation. Visual Cognition, 26(9), 719-733. [22] Satel J., Wilson N. R., & Klein R. M. (2019). What neuroscientific studies tell us about inhibition of return. Vision, 3(4), 58. [23] Theeuwes, J., & Pratt, J. (2003). Inhibition of return spreads across 3-D space. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10(3), 616-620. [24] Vienne C., Masfrand S., Bourdin C., & Vercher J. L. (2020). Depth perception in virtual reality systems: Effect of screen distance, environment richness and display factors. IEEE Access, 8, 29099-29110. [25] Wang B. C., Yan C. Y., Klein R. M., & Wang Z. G. (2018). Inhibition of return revisited: Localized inhibition on top of a pervasive bias. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(5), 1861-1867. [26] Wang, Z. G., & Klein, R. M. (2010). Searching for inhibition of return in visual search: A review. Vision Research, 50(2), 220-228. |