›› 2021, Vol. ›› Issue (1): 60-66.

Previous Articles     Next Articles

The Development of Preschool Children’s Co-representation in Joint Actions

  

  • Received:2020-03-07 Revised:2020-06-23 Online:2021-01-20 Published:2021-01-20
  • Contact: Wen Liu

幼儿在联合行动中共同表征能力的发展

林琳琳1,2,刘文2,王薇薇2,龚平3   

  1. 1. 丽水学院
    2. 辽宁师范大学
    3. 大连市沙河口区教师第二幼儿园
  • 通讯作者: 刘文

Abstract: Joint actions refers to any form of social interaction whereby two or more individuals coordinate their actions in space and time to bring about a change in the environment. At present, there are debates in existing literature about when children have the ability to perform adult-like joint actions because of the difficulty in identifying what mechanisms underlie the behaviors observed in children. In order to coordinate successfully, adults always represent tasks/actions of their own and partners’ in a functionally equivalent way, in which their own performance would be interfered. This interference phenomenon can be used as a index for adult-like joint action in children. The current study investigated whether young children owned the ability of co-representation which brought with interference in joint tasks. In experiment 1, 186 children aged 3~5 years old were chosen randomly to complete Same tasks or Different tasks with experimenter(partner). In the Same task condition, children and their partner had to press mouse button to respond to the same stimulus, while in the Different task condition, they had to respond to two different stimulus. If children co-represent their partner in this joint task, then there should be a cost to performance when the partner is following a different rule, compared with following the same rule. However, if children do not co-represent, the rule their partner is following would not cause interference. The results indicated that there were no significant difference between Same and Different task condition in 3 years old children, while children aged 4 and 5 years old performed worse in Different task than in Same condition. It indicated that 4 years old children might begin to experience interference in Different task. However, a potential alternative explanation for the difference between the two conditions was that participants in the Different task were confused about their task rules, as children heard two different instructions, while in the Same task participants heard the same instruction twice. In order to exclude the potential interpretation, a condition that co-representation of partner’s task might be beneficial, but where confusion due to instructions would be detrimental was designed. In experiment 2, children aged from 4 to 5 years old switched which stimulus to respond to half way through the game. If the children in the Different task were representing their partner’s task in the first half of the experiment, then switch costs should be reduced, as children already had the representation of the opposite task in mind before they switched to it. In contrast, in the Same task, children had no reason to represent the other’s task as both participants shared the same task, thus the opposite task is novel, therefore they experienced more switch costs. The results in pre-switch part replicated the results in experiment 1 and children had less switch costs in the Different task compared with the Same task condition. It meant that the interference children experienced in Different condition was really from co-representation of their own and partner’s task. In general, 3 years old children could not co-represent partner’s task and experience interference in joint task, while 4 and 5 years old children had the ability to co-represent their own and partner’s task in joint actions.

Key words: preschool children, Joint Action, co-representation, development

摘要: 为探究幼儿联合行动共同表征能力的发展,实验1通过比较186名3~5岁幼儿在相同任务和不同任务的表现,发现4岁和5岁幼儿在不同任务中的表现比相同任务差;实验2进一步验证幼儿在不同任务中较差的表现源于对自我和同伴的共同表征。结论:3岁幼儿无法在联合任务中同时表征自我和同伴,没有表现出明显的共同表征能力,4岁以上幼儿开始具备稳定的共同表征能力,能够在联合任务中同时表征自己和同伴,由此对自己的行为表现造成干扰。

关键词: 幼儿 联合行动 共同表征 发展