[1] 蔡海娟. (2018) . 延迟选择过程中自我—他人决策偏好差异研究 (硕士学位论文) . 上海师范大学. [2] 崔楠, 徐岚, 谢雯婷. (2016) . 做会后悔还是不做会后悔?——自我调节模式对不作为惯性的影响. 心理学报, 48(4), 423-434. [3] 黄仁辉, 刘雁伶, 罗小凤, 严蔷薇, 李洁. (2017) . 不确定性容忍度、决策者角色对医疗决策的影响. 中国临床心理学杂志, 25(3), 426-429. [4] 卢长宝, 王传声. (2018) . 过多促销品会导致过多选择效应吗?——时间压力的调节作用. 经济管理, 40(3), 109-127. [5] 陆静怡, 尚雪松. (2018) . 为他人做决策: 多维度心理机制与决策体验. 心理科学进展, 26(9), 1545-1552. [6] 王怀勇, 陈翠萍. (2021) . 调节模式对选择超载的影响及其机制: 一个有中介的调节模型. 心理科学, 44(5), 1057-1063. [7] 吴岩, 高约飞, 赵思敏, 王穗苹. (2019) . 常识性知识和语篇语境对代词指认的影响. 心理学报, 51(3), 293-303. [8] Anderson, C. J. (2003). The psychology of doing nothing: Forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 139-167. [9] Avnet, T., & Higgins, E. T. (2003). Locomotion, assessment, and regulatory fit: Value transfer from "how" to "what". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(5), 525-530. [10] Chang C. C., Chuang S. C., Cheng Y. H., & Huang T. Y. (2012). The compromise effect in choosing for others. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(2), 109-122. [11] Chen C. Y., Rossignac-Milon M., & Higgins E. T. (2018). Feeling distressed from making decisions: Assessors' need to be right. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(4), 743-761. [12] Dhar, R. (1996). The effect of decision strategy on deciding to defer choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 9(4), 265-281. [13] Dhar, R., & Nowlis, S. M. (1999). The effect of time pressure on consumer choice deferral. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(4), 369-384. [14] Etkin, J., & Ghosh, A. P. (2018). When being in a positive mood increases choice deferral. Journal of Consumer Research, 45(1), 208-225. [15] Godinho S., Prada M., & Garrido M. V. (2016). Under pressure: An integrative perspective of time pressure impact on consumer decision-making. Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 28(4), 251-273. [16] Jonas, E., & Frey, D. (2003). Information search and presentation in advisor-client interactions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91(2), 154-168. [17] Kerstholt, J. (1994). The effect of time pressure on decision-making behaviour in a dynamic task environment. Acta Psychologica, 86(1), 89-104. [18] Krijnen J. M. T., Zeelenberg M., & Breugelmans S. M. (2015). Decision importance as a cue for deferral. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(5), 407-415. [19] Kruglanski A. W., Thompson E. P., Higgins E. T., Atash M. N., Pierro A., Shah J. Y., & Spiegel S. (2000). To "do the right thing" or to "just do it": Locomotion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory imperatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 793-815. [20] Larasati, A., & Yeh, M. Y. (2016). Does more attractive choice always decrease choice deferral? The moderating effect of ideal point. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 54, 43-51. [21] Leonhardt J. M., Keller L. R., & Pechmann C. (2011). Avoiding the risk of responsibility by seeking uncertainty: Responsibility aversion and preference for indirect agency when choosing for others. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(4), 405-413. [22] Liu Y., Polman E., Liu Y. F., & Jiao J. L. (2018). Choosing for others and its relation to information search. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 147, 65-75. [23] Luan M., Fu L. S., & Li H. (2018). Do maximizers maximize for others? Self-other decision-making differences in maximizing and satisficing. Personality and Individual Differences, 121, 52-56. [24] Mugon J., Struk A., & Danckert J. (2018). A failure to launch: Regulatory modes and boredom proneness. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1126. [25] Nagpal A., Khare A., Chowdhury T., Labrecque L. I., & Pandit A. (2011). The impact of the amount of available information on decision delay: The role of common features. Marketing Letters, 22(4), 405-421. [26] Nordbye G. H. H., Riege A. H., & Teigen K. H. (2018). Better safe than sorry: Risking irresponsibility by seeking uncertainty. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31(1), 87-99. [27] Orehek E., Mauro R., Kruglanski A. W., & van der Bles, A. M. (2012). Prioritizing association strength versus value: The influence of self-regulatory modes on means evaluation in single goal and multigoal contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(1), 22-31. [28] Pierro A., Giacomantonio M., Pica G., Kruglanski A. W., & Higgins E. T. (2011). On the psychology of time in action: Regulatory mode orientations and procrastination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1317-1331. [29] Pilli, L. E., & Mazzon, J. A. (2016). Information overload, choice deferral, and moderating role of need for cognition: Empirical evidence. Revista de Administração, 51(1), 36-55. [30] Rassin, E., & Muris, P. (2005). Indecisiveness and the interpretation of ambiguous situations. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(7), 1285-1291. [31] Sela A., Berger J., & Liu W. (2009). Variety, vice, and virtue: How assortment size influences option choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(6), 941-951. [32] Shortland N., Alison L., & Thompson L. (2020). Military maximizers: Examining the effect of individual differences in maximization on military decision-making. Personality and Individual Differences, 163, Article 110051. [33] Steffel M., Williams E. F., & Perrmann-Graham J. (2016). Passing the buck: Delegating choices to others to avoid responsibility and blame. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 135, 32-44. [34] Wang, H. Y., & Liu, Y. F. (2016). The impact of regulatory fit on postdecision evaluation. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 44(3), 383-390. [35] Wang Z. J., Kuang Y., Tang H. Y., Gao C., Chen A., & Chan K. Q. (2018). Are decisions made by group representatives more risk averse? The effect of sense of responsibility. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31(3), 311-323. [36] Zhang C. Y., Ni Y., & Feng T. Y. (2017). The effect of regulatory mode on procrastination: Bi-stable parahippocampus connectivity with dorsal anterior cingulate and anterior prefrontal cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 329, 51-57. |